Why Depressed People See Ghosts

Depressed People See GhostsDepressed people are more likely to see ghosts according to a study of 85 female college students participated in recently. Each participant completed a psychokinesis (PK) task and completed the magical ideation scale. Consistent with research reported by Thalbourne and others (e.g., Thalbourne & Delin, 1994), particpants who had a higher level of depression also showed higher illusory control scores.

In a second study of 105 participants completed a preconition task and a PK task as well as a judgement of contingency task used by Alloy and Abramson (1979) to test to the so-called “depressive realism” hypothesis. This study replicated the findings of the first study. Both studies show that there is a relation between magical thinking and psychopathology

There is much research that suggests people who experience paranormal phenomena such as Ghosts and UFO experiences suffer from certain forms of psychopathology. It has been well documented that people who suffer from schizophrenia were high in magical ideation which Meehl (cited in Echblad and CHapman, 1983) defined as “belief, quasi-belief, or semi-serious entertainment of the possiblity that events which, according to causal concepts of this culture, cannot have a causal relation to each other, might somehow nevertheless doso” (quote from Eckblad & CHapman, 1983, p. 215).

Magical Ideation Scale

In 1983 scientists Edkblad and Chapman developed a self-report scale to measure “magical ideation”. Items assessed belief in the following:

•thought transmission
•psychokinesis
•precognition
•astrology
•other dimensions

A group of students attending a large university here in the US were given the test. The students who scored high on the Magical Ideation scale were studied furthre. IT was discovered that most of these students reported more psychotic and psychotic-like symptoms, schizotypal experiences and mood symptoms (depressed and manic) than those who scored low on magical ideation.

In Australia, a scientist named Thalbourne and his colleagues found a number of intriguing relations among the levels of magical ideation, paranormal belief and symptoms of psychopathology (e.g. Thalbourne, Bartemucci, Delin, Fox, & Nofi, 1997;Thalbourne and French, 1995). For example, Thalbourne and Delin found that the level of magical ideation and paranormal belief were positively correlated with the extent of the manic experience, depressive experience, manic depressive experience, and hypo-mania.

In conclusion, it easy to suggest that there is a vast amount of research that points to the positive relation between the extent of people’s belief in magical causes (including paranormal phenomena) and the level of certain symptoms associated with psychopathology. People who experience higher levels of psychopathology are more likely to believe in the reality of causal connections between events that are not, according to generally accepted concepts, casually connected.


REFERENCES

Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441-485.

Alloy, L. B., & Clements, C. M. (1992). Illusion of control: Invulnerability to negative affect and depressive symptoms after laboratory and natural Stressors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 234-245.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington: Author.

Ayeroff, F., & Abelson, R. P. (1976). ESP and ESB: Belief in personal success at mental telepathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 240-247.

Benassi, V. A., & Belli, R. F. (1989). Statistics and replication experiments: Comparing interaction effects. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13, 283-294.

Benassi, V. A., & Mahler, H. I. (1985). Contingency judgments by depressed college students: Sadder but not always wiser. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1323-1329.

Benassi, V. A., Sweeney, P. D., & Drevno, G. E. (1979). Mind over matter: Perceived success at psychokinesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1377-1386.

Bryson, S. E., Doan, B. D., & Pasquali, P. (1984). Sadder bust wiser: A failure to demonstrate that mood influences judgments of control. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 65, 145-153.

Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., Kwapil, T. R., Eckblad, M., & Zinser, M.C. (1994). Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 171-183.

deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.

Eckblad, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 51, 215-225.

George, L., & Neufeld, R. W. (1987). Magical ideation and schizophrenia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 778-779.

Irwin, H. J., & Green, M. J. (1998-1999). Schizotypal processes and belief in the paranormal: A multidimensional study. European Journal of Parapsychology, 14, 1-15.

Kwapil, T. R., Miller, M. B., Zinser, M. C., Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J., & Eckblad, M. (1994). A longitudinal study of high scorers on the hypomanic personality scale. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 222-236.

Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311-328.

Langer, E. J., & Roth, J. (1975). Heads I win, tails it’s chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 951-955.

Martin, D. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1984). The illusion of control for self and others in depressed and nondepressed college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 125-136.

Peltzer, K. (2002). Paranormal beliefs and personality among black South African students. Social Behavior and Personality, 30, 391-398.

Plutchik, R., Platman, S. R., Tilles, R., & Fieve, R. R. (1970). Construction and evaluation of a test for measuring mania and depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 499-503.

Presson, P. K., & Benassi, V. A. (1996). Illusion of control: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 493-510.

Thalbourne, M. A. (1998). Technical note: The level of paranormal belief and experience among psychotics. Journal of Parapsychology, 62, 79-81.

Thalbourne, M. A., Bartemucci, L., Delin, P. S., Fox, B., & Nofi, O. (1997). Transliminality: Its nature and correlates. The Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 91, 305-331.

Thalbourne, M. A, & Delin, P. S. (1994). A common thread underlying belief in the paranormal, creative personality, mystical experience, and psychopathology. Journal of Parapsychology, 58, 3-38.

Thalbourne, M. A., & French, C. C. (1995). Paranormal belief, manic-depressiveness, and magical ideation: A replication. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 291-292.

Thalbourne, M. A., Keogh, E., & Crawley, S.E. (1999). Manic-depressiveness and its correlates. Psychological Reports, 85, 45-53.

World Health Organization. (1992). The International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems. 10th revised ed. Geneva: Author.

Zusne, L., & Jones, W.H. (1989). Anomalistic psychology: A study of magical thinking. (2nd ed.) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *